生物谷報(bào)道:大多數(shù)孩子都習(xí)慣于這樣的心理游戲:當(dāng)被要求為了允諾的款待——例如觀看1小時(shí)電視——而耐心等待時(shí),這些孩子往往會(huì)用一個(gè)玩具或一本書轉(zhuǎn)移自己的注意力,。研究人員如今發(fā)現(xiàn),,黑猩猩也具有類似的注意力自我分散行為。這一結(jié)果使人與其他靈長類動(dòng)物之間的認(rèn)知與行為邊界變得愈發(fā)模糊不清,。
這一發(fā)現(xiàn)來自美國亞特蘭大市佐治亞州立大學(xué)完成的一項(xiàng)研究。心理學(xué)家Theodore Evans和Michael Beran分別將4只黑猩猩置于一個(gè)與糖果自動(dòng)售貨機(jī)相連的容器前。這些靈長類動(dòng)物能夠在任何時(shí)間接近這個(gè)容器,,并吃掉其中堆積的糖果,然而一旦黑猩猩開始享用糖果,,自動(dòng)售貨機(jī)便不再補(bǔ)給任何新的糖果,。這就使得黑猩猩盡可能地拖延它們獲得獎(jiǎng)賞的時(shí)間,從而使自己能夠吃到更多的糖果,。
在另一項(xiàng)試驗(yàn)中,,這些黑猩猩被安排在類似的場景中,這次,,研究人員還為它們提供了玩具,。像坐立不安的孩子一樣,在這種情況下,,黑猩猩通過將注意力轉(zhuǎn)移到玩具上,,能夠堅(jiān)持更長的時(shí)間。研究小組在日前的《生物學(xué)快報(bào)》網(wǎng)絡(luò)版上報(bào)告了這一研究成果,。為了確定玩玩具是否真的分散了黑猩猩的注意力,,研究人員為黑猩猩設(shè)計(jì)了另外一種場景——它們能夠看到盛滿糖果的容器,但卻不能接近后者,。與無法觸碰容器相比,,在能夠接觸容器的情況下,大多數(shù)的黑猩猩都會(huì)花更多的時(shí)間玩玩具,,這意味著它們的游戲是一種權(quán)衡的策略,,為的是控制自己吃糖果的沖動(dòng)。
佐治亞州亞特蘭大市恩莫利大學(xué)的靈長類動(dòng)物學(xué)家Frans de Waal表示,,這項(xiàng)研究填補(bǔ)了人類與其他物種之間的又一個(gè)“心理缺口”,,同時(shí)為令人印象深刻的動(dòng)物認(rèn)知技能增加了新的內(nèi)容,例如使用工具以及計(jì)劃未來,。他指出:“對于人類而言,,我們會(huì)將這種行為視為一種與誘惑有關(guān)的自我意識(shí),即除非你轉(zhuǎn)移自己的注意力,,否則你將墜入其中,。”
作者表示,,這項(xiàng)研究同時(shí)說明動(dòng)物的自我意識(shí)比科學(xué)家之前認(rèn)識(shí)的要復(fù)雜得多。Evans說:“它讓我們思考,,對于黑猩猩和其他動(dòng)物的心理活動(dòng)到底有多少了解,。”他指出:“黑猩猩能夠意識(shí)到它們正在被糖果誘惑嗎?與此類似,,人類是否意識(shí)到誘惑他們的往往是一些多余的商品呢,?”(科學(xué)時(shí)報(bào))
原始出處:
Biology Letters
Researchers have found that chimps know how to distract themselves with play in order to ward off temptation.
Chimpanzees use self-distraction to cope with impulsivity
Theodore A. Evans1, Michael J. Beran1
1Language Research Center, Georgia State University, University Plaza, Atlanta, GA 30302, USA
摘要
It is unknown whether animals, like humans, can employ behavioural strategies to cope with impulsivity. To examine this question, we tested whether chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) would use self-distraction as a coping strategy in a situation in which they had to continually inhibit responses to accumulating candies in order to earn a greater amount of those rewards. We tested animals in three conditions in which they were sometimes given a set of toys and were sometimes allowed physical access to the accumulating candies. Chimpanzees allowed the rewards to accumulate longer before responding when they could divert their attention to the toys, and they manipulated the toys more when the candies were physically accessible. Thus, chimpanzees engaged in self-distraction with the toys when such behaviour was most beneficial as a coping mechanism.
Keywords
self-distraction, self-control, impulsivity, chimpanzee, Pan troglodytes
References
Ainslie, G.W. 1974 Impulse control in pigeons. J. Exp. Anal. Behav. 21, 485–489, (doi:10.1901/jeab.1974.21-485). [CrossRef]
Aureli, F. & de Waal, F.B.M. 1997 Inhibition of social behavior in chimpanzees under high-density conditions. Am. J. Primatol. 41, 213–228, (doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2345(1997)41:3<213::AID-AJP4>3.0.CO;2-#). [CrossRef]
Beran, M.J. 2002 Maintenance of self-imposed delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and an orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus). J. Gen. Psychol. 129, 49–66.
Beran, M.J. & Evans, T.A. 2006 Maintenance of delay of gratification by four chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): the role of delayed reward visibility, experimenter presence, and extended delay intervals. Behav. Processes 73, 315–324, (doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2006.07.005). [CrossRef]
Beran, M.J., Savage-Rumbaugh, E.S., Pate, J.L. & Rumbaugh, D.M. 1999 Delay of gratification in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Dev. Psychobiol. 34, 119–127, (doi:10.1002/(SICI)1098-2302(199903)34:2<119::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-P). [CrossRef]
Leavens, D.A., Aurelli, F., Hopkins, W.D. & Hyatt, C.W. 2001 Effects of cognitive challenge on self-directed behaviors by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Am. J. Primatol. 55, 1–14, (doi:10.1002/ajp.1034). [CrossRef]
Logue, A.W. 1988 Research on self-control: an integrating framework. Behav. Brain Sci. 11, 665–709.
Miller, D.T. & Karniol, R. 1976 The role of rewards in externally and self-imposed delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 34, 310–316, (doi:10.1037/0022-3514.34.2.310). [CrossRef]
Mischel, W., Ebbesen, E.B. & Zeiss, A.R. 1972 Cognitive and attentional mechanisms in delay of gratification. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 21, 204–218, (doi:10.1037/h0032198). [CrossRef]
Rachlin, H. 2000 The science of self-control. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
Stevens, J.R., Rosati, A.G., Ross, K.R. & Hauser, M.D. 2005 Will travel for food: spatial discounting in two New World monkeys. Curr. Biol. 15, 1855–1860, (doi:10.1016/j.cub.2005.09.016). [CrossRef]
Tobin, H. & Logue, A.W. 1994 Self-control across species (Columbia livia, Homo sapiens, and Rattus norvegicus). J. Comp. Psychol. 108, 126–133, (doi:10.1037/0735-7036.108.2.126). [CrossRef]
Toner, I.J. & Smith, R.A. 1977 Age and overt verbalization in delay-maintenance behavior in children. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 24, 123–128, (doi:10.1016/0022-0965(77)90025-X). [CrossRef]