北京時(shí)間4月21日消息,據(jù)英國(guó)《每日郵報(bào)》報(bào)道,,英國(guó)的科學(xué)家日前稱,,他們最近在研究中發(fā)現(xiàn)鳥類其實(shí)和人類的軍隊(duì)非常相似,在覓食或者睡覺時(shí)也會(huì)派遣哨兵“站崗”,,當(dāng)發(fā)現(xiàn)危險(xiǎn)時(shí),,這些哨兵會(huì)發(fā)出獨(dú)特的鳴叫以對(duì)同伴進(jìn)行提醒和報(bào)警。
在最新一期的《當(dāng)代生物學(xué)》(Current Biology)雜志上,,研究人員們將鳥類的這一行為比作在敵方領(lǐng)土上的值班士兵,,他們會(huì)定期與同伴們進(jìn)行無線電聯(lián)絡(luò),以確保其安全,。研究稱,,自然選擇學(xué)說認(rèn)為,單個(gè)個(gè)體為了生存,,會(huì)表現(xiàn)出自私的行為,,因此,此次最新發(fā)現(xiàn)向人們展示了一個(gè)動(dòng)物世界中罕有的合作行為特例,。研究人員安迪·拉德弗德表示,,此次研究的鳥類是非洲斑鹛,它們通過這種“崗哨”方法來保證正在覓食伙伴們能夠?qū)P摹踩厥占嗟氖澄?。他說:“鳥兒們也從這種無私的崗哨行為中獲益匪淺,,它們贏得了個(gè)體生存的良好環(huán)境,進(jìn)而形成了更龐大的種族群落,。當(dāng)整個(gè)群體受到攻擊或者被迫退敵于領(lǐng)土之外時(shí),,這一行為也增加了哨兵的生存機(jī)會(huì)。這些研究結(jié)果令人十分激動(dòng),,這是真正合作的絕好例證,,展現(xiàn)了一個(gè)雙贏的局面。”
斑鹛生長(zhǎng)在南非,,一般以6到7個(gè)個(gè)體為一組生活在一起,,它們常常以捕食藏于地表下的蝎子和小蛇為生。通過研究喀拉哈里沙漠中的斑鹛群,,研究人員們表示,,無論是否能看見樹中的哨兵,覓食者都會(huì)對(duì)其緊急號(hào)做出響應(yīng),。有了這種緊急號(hào),,覓食者就可以不用尋找哨兵的身影而持續(xù)覓食,這就增加了覓食時(shí)間,,并提高了整個(gè)鳥群的存活率,。通過回放緊急號(hào)錄間,研究人員們發(fā)現(xiàn),,覓食的鳥兒會(huì)更廣地分散開來,,花在抬頭觀察有無天敵的時(shí)間上更上,而花在野外覓食的時(shí)間上更多,。
拉德弗德說:“決定對(duì)聲音線索作出回應(yīng)是群居鳥類中很重要的一種行為,,通過研究這種行為,我們能夠更多地了解不同種群的動(dòng)物在語言使用上的進(jìn)化方式,。目前,我們正在研究哨兵的可靠性是否會(huì)有不同,,以及這種差異是否會(huì)影響種群中的伙伴,。此項(xiàng)研究共耗資30萬英磅,主要由英國(guó)生物技術(shù)與生物科學(xué)研究委員會(huì)提供資助,。”
此前的研究證實(shí),,黑頭山雀的叫聲是已知最復(fù)雜的鳥類警報(bào)系統(tǒng)之一。當(dāng)一只掠食者逼近時(shí),,黑頭山雀會(huì)發(fā)出它們熟悉的“chick·a·dee·dee·dee”的叫聲,。其中“dee”音的數(shù)量與掠食者的大小和相對(duì)威脅成正比。為了搞清其他鳥類是否也能破譯這些救命的叫聲,,科學(xué)家們?cè)鴮?duì)紅胸幣鳥(黑頭山雀的老鄰居)進(jìn)行了追蹤觀測(cè),。研究人員在紅胸幣鳥棲息的樹林中安放了一些揚(yáng)聲器,,但是這里并沒有黑頭山雀的蹤跡。他們隨后分別播放了黑頭山雀對(duì)于一種小而敏捷的貓頭鷹和大而笨拙的美洲雕鸮的不同預(yù)警叫聲,,并觀察紅胸幣鳥的反應(yīng),。科學(xué)家通過20次試驗(yàn)后發(fā)現(xiàn),,與聽到關(guān)于美洲雕鸮的警告相比,,在聽到有關(guān)小貓頭鷹的警報(bào)后,會(huì)有兩倍數(shù)量的紅胸幣鳥表現(xiàn)出明顯的圍攻行為——它們聚集在揚(yáng)聲器周圍,,激動(dòng)地拍打著翅膀,,并且發(fā)出自己的警告叫聲。研究人員們表示,,如果威脅相對(duì)較低,,紅胸幣鳥并不會(huì)浪費(fèi)自己寶貴的能量。(來源:新浪科技 劉妍)
生物谷推薦原始出處:
(Current Biology),,doi:10.1016/j.cub.2008.02.078,,Linda I. Hollén, Andrew N. Radford
Cooperative Sentinel Calling? Foragers Gain Increased Biomass Intake
Linda I. Hollén1, Matthew B.V. Bell2 and Andrew N. Radford1, ,
Summary
Many foraging animals face a fundamental tradeoff between predation and starvation [1] and [2]. In a range of social species, this tradeoff has probably driven the evolution of sentinel behavior, where individuals adopt prominent positions to watch for predators while groupmates forage [3]. Although there has been much debate about whether acting as a sentinel is a selfish or cooperative behavior [3], [4], [5] and [6], far less attention has focused on why sentinels often produce quiet vocalizations (hereafter known as “sentinel calls”) to announce their presence [7] and [8]. We use observational and experimental data to provide the first evidence that group members gain an increase in foraging success by responding to these vocal cues given by sentinels. Foraging pied babblers (Turdoides bicolor) spread out more, use more exposed patches, look up less often, and spend less time vigilant in response to sentinel calling. Crucially, we demonstrate that these behavioral alterations lead to an increase in biomass intake by foragers, which is likely to enhance survival. We argue that this benefit may be the reason for sentinel calling, making it a truly cooperative behavior